Newer
Older
Publications / Gleeson_paper.tex
% Template article for preprint document class `elsart'
% with harvard style bibliographic references
% SP 2001/01/05

\documentclass{elsart}

% Use the option doublespacing or reviewcopy to obtain double line spacing
% \documentclass[doublespacing]{elsart}

% the natbib package allows both number and author-year (Harvard)
% style referencing;
\usepackage[authoryear,sort]{natbib}

% if you use PostScript figures in your article
% use the graphics package for simple commands
% \usepackage{graphics}
% or use the graphicx package for more complicated commands
\usepackage{graphicx}
% or use the epsfig package if you prefer to use the old commands
% \usepackage{epsfig}

% The amssymb package provides various useful mathematical symbols
\usepackage{amssymb}


\usepackage{units}
\usepackage{url}


\begin{document}

\begin{frontmatter}

% Title, authors and addresses

% use the thanksref command within \title, \author or \address for footnotes;
% use the corauthref command within \author for corresponding author footnotes;
% use the ead command for the email address,
% and the form \ead[url] for the home page:
% \title{Title\thanksref{label1}}
% \thanks[label1]{}
% \author{Name\corauthref{cor1}\thanksref{label2}}
% \ead{email address}
% \ead[url]{home page}
% \thanks[label2]{}
% \corauth[cor1]{}
% \address{Address\thanksref{label3}}
% \thanks[label3]{}

\title{Assessing the efficacy of a touch screen overlay as a selection
device for typical GUI targets}

% use optional labels to link authors explicitly to addresses:
% \author[label1,label2]{}
% \address[label1]{}
% \address[label2]{}

\author[info]{Matthew Gleeson},
\ead{matt\_gleeson@xtra.co.nz}
\author[info]{Nigel Stanger\corauthref{cor}},
\ead{nstanger@infoscience.otago.ac.nz}
\corauth[cor]{Corresponding author. Tel.: +64-3-479-8179; fax: +64-3-479-8311}
\author[hunt]{Elaine Ferguson}
\ead{elaine.ferguson@stonebow.otago.ac.nz}

\address[info]{Department of Information Science,}
\address[hunt]{Department of Human Nutrition, \\ University of Otago, PO Box 56, Dunedin, New Zealand}


\begin{abstract}

In this paper we investigate the efficacy of a touch screen overlay
compared to a mouse, when selecting typical graphical user interface
(GUI) items in a desktop information system. A series of tests were
completed involving multi-directional point and select tasks, and the
results for both devices compared. The results showed that the touch
screen overlay was not suitable for selecting GUI targets smaller than
\unit[4]{mm}. The touch screen overlay was slower and had a higher error
rate than the mouse, but there was no significant difference in
throughput. Testers rated the mouse easier to use and to make accurate
selections, while the touch screen overlay resulted in greater arm,
wrist and finger fatigue. These results suggest that a touch screen
overlay is not a practical selection device for desktop interfaces with
small GUI targets.

\end{abstract}

\begin{keyword}
Touch screen overlay \sep
Mouse \sep
Selection device \sep
Fitts' Law \sep
Performance evaluation \sep
GUI items
\end{keyword}

\end{frontmatter}

% main text
\newcommand{\ISOnine}{ISO 9241-9}


\section{Introduction}
\label{sec-introduction}

Most modern information systems that run on desktop personal computers
are designed to be used with a keyboard and mouse. While the combination
of keyboard and mouse is the accepted method of interaction with such
systems it does not necessarily suit all information systems.
Information systems with limited data entry may be more usable through
the use of a keyboard and touch screen. Touch screens require less
physical space and thus the workstation environment in an office setting
could be improved, allocating more space to the user and less to the
computer.

The purpose of this paper is to investigate how effective a touch screen
overlay is compared to a mouse, when selecting typical graphical user
interface (GUI) items used in information systems. The target types
tested were buttons, check boxes, combo boxes and text boxes, which are
typical of those found in an interface for an information system. Each
target type was tested at three different sizes (see
Section~\ref{sec-GUI}).

A typical touch screen device comprises a monitor enhanced with hardware
for detecting touches on the screen surface. An alternative approach is
to attach a discrete touch-sensitive surface to an existing conventional
monitor. \citet{Sear-A-1991-IJMMS} have previously assessed the efficacy
of specialised touch screen hardware, but there appears to have been
little research into the efficacy of touch screen overlays. We therefore
chose to compare the performance of a touch screen overlay with that of
a mouse.

The testing occurred in the context of a research project undertaken by
the Department of Human Nutrition at the University of Otago. This
project aimed to improve complementary food diets for toddlers in New
Zealand, by designing a computer program to help formulate
population-specific food-based dietary guidelines for this high risk
group. The program was a rapid assessment decision-making tool, designed
specifically to assist nutrition programme planners in selecting
appropriate and improved home-based complementary foods
\citep{Ferg-E-2004}. The testing for our study took place within this
program environment.

% do we need this?
The remainder of the paper discusses the experimental design and the
results obtained. Section~\ref{sec-GUI} briefly describes the types of
targets used in the experiment, while Section~\ref{sec-evaluation}
describes the measures used to evaluate the selection devices. The
experimental design is described in Section~\ref{sec-method}.
Section~\ref{sec-analysis} describes how the data were analysed, and
Section~\ref{sec-results} presents the results of the experiment. Our
conclusions are presented in Section~\ref{sec-conclusions}.


\section{GUI targets}
\label{sec-GUI}

Since the 1980's much work has gone into developing human computer
interface guidelines. Today's interfaces are made up of a combination of
different targets that include text boxes, check boxes, combo boxes,
list boxes, buttons, labels, tool bars, etc. \citet{Sear-A-1991-IJMMS}
showed that touch screens can be successfully used as a selection device
and can have advantages over a mouse, even for small targets. These
results were, however, based on selecting arbitrary shapes and not the
typical targets found in modern GUIs.

To accurately test the performance of each selection device within the
experiment, three different sizes of GUI target were used, corresponding
to small, medium and large GUI items. As our experiment took place in a
Windows environment, we would have preferred to base these sizes on
Microsoft's user interface guidelines. However, Microsoft's guidelines
specify only a single standard size for most GUI items \citep[pp.\
448--450]{MS-1999-UI}, and also measure the sizes of these items in a
resolution-independent manner, rather than by specifying fixed sizes. We
have therefore adopted the three standard sizes (mini, small and large)
specified by \citeauthor{Appl-2004-HIG}'s \citeyearpar{Appl-2004-HIG}
human interface guidelines, which are listed in
Table~\ref{tab-target-sizes}.


\begin{table}
	\caption{Target sizes (width \(\times\) height) used in the experiment.}
	\label{tab-target-sizes}
	\begin{tabular}{llll}
		\hline
		\textbf{Target type}	&	\textbf{Large}							&	\textbf{Medium}							&	\textbf{Small}	\\
		\hline
		Text box				&	\(\unit[63]{mm} \times \unit[11]{mm}\)	&	\(\unit[55]{mm} \times \unit[8]{mm}\)	&	\(\unit[47]{mm} \times \unit[6]{mm}\)	\\
		Combo box				&	\(\unit[63]{mm} \times \unit[11]{mm}\)	&	\(\unit[55]{mm} \times \unit[8]{mm}\)	&	\(\unit[47]{mm} \times \unit[6]{mm}\)	\\
		Button					&	\(\unit[28]{mm} \times \unit[13]{mm}\)	&	\(\unit[24]{mm} \times \unit[9]{mm}\)	&	\(\unit[17]{mm} \times \unit[6]{mm}\)	\\
		Check box\(^{a}\)		&	\(\unit[9]{mm} \times \unit[9]{mm}\)	&	\(\unit[6]{mm} \times \unit[6]{mm}\)	&	\(\unit[4]{mm} \times \unit[4]{mm}\)	\\
		\hline
	\end{tabular}
	
	{\footnotesize \(^{a}\)This refers to the size of the check box itself, not the associated text label.}
\end{table}


\section{Evaluation methods}
\label{sec-evaluation}

Each selection device was assessed using a combination of performance
and comfort measures. The performance measures were primarily taken from
the \ISOnine\ standard \citep{ISO-2000-9241-9}, while the comfort
measures were derived from a questionnaire administered to test
participants.


\subsection{Performance}
\label{sec-evaluation-performance}

ISO 9241 specifies standards for the ergonomic design of office
computing environments. Part 9 of this standard describes different
tests that can be used to evaluate one or more pointing devices
\citep{ISO-2000-9241-9}. The standard describes a serial point and
select task and recognises a dependent measure used with this test,
known as \emph{throughput}. The serial test comprises moving the cursor
back and forth between two targets using the pointing device and
selecting each target by pressing and releasing a button on the pointing
device. One disadvantage of this approach is that only two targets are
used in the test and therefore interactions between more than two
targets, which often happen in a typical information system interface,
are not studied.

\citet{Mack-IS-2001-EHCI} note that throughput
is a very important measure, as it reflects the efficiency of the user
completing the task and is a measure of both speed and
accuracy. Throughput is calculated by the following formula:
\begin{equation}
	\label{eqn-throughput}
	\mathit{throughput} = \frac{\mathit{ID}_{e}}{\mathit{MT}}
\end{equation}
where \(\mathit{MT}\) is the movement time in seconds (defined as time
taken to successfully select a target) and \(\mathit{ID}_{e}\) is Fitts'
\citeyearpar{Fitt-PM-1954-Law} \emph{index of difficulty} measured in
bits. Throughput is thus measured in bits per second (bps).

The index of difficulty is calculated by the following formula:
\begin{equation}
	\label{eqn-IDe}
	\mathit{ID}_{e} = \log_{2}\left(\frac{D}{W_{e}} + 1\right)
\end{equation}
where \(D\) is the distance to the target and \(W_{e}\) is the
\emph{effective width} of the target.

The effective width \(W_{e}\) reflects spatial variability in a sequence
of trials, and thus differs from the actual width of the target. The
effective width of a target is calculated by the following formula:
\begin{equation}
	\label{eqn-We}
	W_{e} = 4.133 \times \mathrm{SD}_{x}
\end{equation}
where \(\mathit{SD}_{x}\) is the standard deviation in the selection
coordinates measured along the path to target \(x\).

\ISOnine\ does not provide any guidance on the range of index of
difficulty values to use in testing. \citet{Doug-SA-1999-CHI} recommend
using a range from 2 to 6 bits. They also recommend calculating the
\emph{error rate} as a separate dependent measure of accuracy. The
error rate is defined as the ratio of incorrect to correct selections
made on a target, so an error rate of 100\% implies that there were as
many errors made as correct selections. Error rate is not included in
\ISOnine, but has been used in several other studies
\citep{Sear-A-1991-IJMMS,Sear-A-1993-BIT,Hara-H-1996,Bend-G-1999-PhD,
Doug-SA-1999-CHI,Mack-IS-2001-EHCI,Po-BA-2004-CHI}. Computing both
throughput and error rate gives a more detailed performance analysis for
the selection device in question.


\subsection{Comfort}
\label{sec-evaluation-comfort}

ISO 9241-9 argues that to fully evaluate a selection device requires
assessment of user effort and comfort in addition to performance
measurements. Comfort is subjective and can be assessed by means of
questionnaires, while effort can be evaluated objectively by measuring
the biomechanical load on users as they use a device. Unfortunately,
such measurements require reasonably sophisticated equipment
\citep{Doug-SA-1999-CHI} that was not available to us. We therefore
omitted effort measurements from our experiment.

A questionnaire was used to assess comfort and user satisfaction for
each selection device in our experiment. The selection device assessment
questionnaire comprised sixteen questions, eight of which were taken
from the ISO ``Independent Questionnaire for Assessment of Comfort''
\citep{Doug-SA-1999-CHI}. The remaining eight questions related
specifically to the target types and target sizes that were tested. In
particular, the questionnaire aimed to assess the participants' comfort
in using the selection device, the difficulty in accurately selecting each
of the target types and the preferred size of each target type using the
selection device.

The responses to twelve of the questions were based on a five point
ordinal scale. The remaining four questions referred to the
participant's preferred size for each target type and were based on a
three point response corresponding to the target sizes tested---small,
medium and large (see Table~\ref{tab-target-sizes}). There was also a
space for participants to provide additional general feedback about the
testing process.


\subsection{Other considerations}
\label{sec-evaluation-other}

\citet{Doug-SA-1999-CHI} also note that \ISOnine\ does not take into
account any possible effects of learning, which can affect movement time
and accuracy. For example, \citet{Mack-IS-1991} found that the movement
times from the first of five testing sessions were significantly higher
than in later sessions. This can be explained as a result of learning
and shows that input device studies should take into account and test
for learning; indeed, \citet{Doug-SA-1999-CHI} recommend applying a
repeated measures paradigm and testing for learning effects.

One interesting aspect of using typical GUI items as targets is the
variation in selection behaviour for different target types, compared to
earlier studies that used simple rectangular targets. The button, check
box and text box target types can be said to employ a ``one-step''
selection behaviour, because they require only single action (i.e., the
user clicks on them) in order to be selected. A combo box is different,
however, because it employs a ``two-step'' selection behaviour: first
the combo box must be selected in order to show the list of items, and
then an item must be selected from the displayed list. This behaviour is
illustrated in Figure~\ref{fig-combo-box}. To complicate matters
further, users may execute this two-step behaviour using either a
``one-click'' or a ``two-click'' approach. In the former approach, the
user clicks on the combo box, drags down to the desired list item, then
releases. In the latter approach, the user clicks once on the combo box,
then clicks again on the desired list item. If the list were longer than
what could be displayed on screen, this could even lead to a
``multiple-click'' approach, where the user clicks multiple times on the
downward scroll arrow in the drop-down list. We have, however, not
considered this possibility in our experiment.


\begin{figure}
	\centering
	\includegraphics[scale=0.8]{combobox-step1}\quad
	\includegraphics[scale=0.8]{combobox-step2}
	\caption{The two-step action required to select a combo box.}
	\label{fig-combo-box}
\end{figure}



\section{Method}
\label{sec-method}

An experiment was carried out to test the effect of size for different
GUI target types with different selection devices. The experiment
involved participants completing a series of simple point and select
tasks. Small, medium and large sizes were tested for a combo box, text
box, check box and button, using either a touch screen overlay or a
mouse. The test was multi-directional, meaning the targets appeared in
multiple directions from the initial starting point. A variety of
different sizes, angles and distances were used for each target
position.

The test itself comprised a screen containing a button in the middle and
a target for the participant to select as illustrated in
Figure~\ref{fig-test-environment}. When a participant clicked on the
centre (``Go'') button, a trial was started and a target appeared on the
screen. The trial ended when the participant successfully clicked the
target, which caused it to disappear. The time taken between clicking
the ``Go'' button and successfully clicking on the target was recorded
as well as the number of errors made during the trial. The final
coordinates of the successful click on the target were recorded in order
to calculate the effective width of the target.


\begin{figure}
	\centering
 	\includegraphics[draft]{test-environment}
	\caption{Screenshot of the test environment with the target in the
	top left of the screen and the ``Go'' button in the centre.}
	\label{fig-test-environment}
\end{figure}


\subsection{Participants}
\label{sec-method-participants}

A participant sample size of twenty-four was used for the experiment.
Each participant was allocated to one of two groups with each group
using one selection device in testing.

The allocation of groups was based upon the results of a questionnaire
completed by each participant prior to testing. The purpose of the
pre-test questionnaire was to establish the level of computer, mouse and
touch screen experience of each participant. Each participant was then
allocated to a selection device group depending on which device they had
the least experience with.

Due to the testing being done within the nutrition program environment
mentioned in Section~\ref{sec-introduction}, the participants were all
nutritionists (i.e., typical users of the program). There were
twenty-one female and three male participants, all with a university
level of education. All participants were unpaid volunteers.


\subsection{Apparatus}
\label{sec-method-apparatus}

The test environment was implemented in Visual Basic.NET using Microsoft
Studio 2003, and is illustrated in Figure~\ref{fig-test-environment}.
During each test, data corresponding to the relevant measures (movement
time, number of errors and selection coordinates) were captured by the
software and automatically written to a Microsoft Excel worksheet.

The touch screen used in testing was a 17'' Magic Touch USB overlay
Model KTMT-1700-USB-M. This device uses a take-off touch strategy, that
is, a selection is not confirmed until the user's finger is removed from
the screen. An important property of touch screen overlays is that they
are placed over a conventional monitor and the touch surface is thus not
coincident with the display surface. This can cause a slight discrepancy
or parallax effect between where the user touches the overlay and where
the cursor is positioned on the screen.

The touch screen overlay was fitted to a Dell 15'' Flat Panel Model
E151FPb monitor. A flat panel monitor was chosen because it was noticed
during pre-testing that typical CRT monitors with curved screens
produced a variable gap between the overlay and the display surface,
thus potentially leading to a greater parallax effect than with a flat
display surface.

The mouse used in testing was a standard Dell PS/2 Optical Mouse Model
M071KC. Both devices were connected to a Dell Inspiron 7500 laptop
computer that ran the testing software.


\subsection{Design}
\label{sec-method-design}

A mixed design experiment was used with the selection device as a
between-subjects factor. The independent (between-subject) variables
were:
\begin{itemize}

	\item Target type (text box, combo box, button and check box)

	\item Target size (large, medium and small)

	\item Target distance (\unit[40]{mm}, \unit[80]{mm} and
	\unit[160]{mm}---see below)

	\item Target angle (\(45^{\circ}\), \(135^{\circ}\), \(225^{\circ}\)
	and \(315^{\circ}\)---see below)

	\item Trial (1 to 144)

	\item Block (1 to 6)

\end{itemize}
The dependent variables within the experiment were throughput, movement
time and error rate.

The entire test was divided into six blocks. Each block contained every
possible combination of target type (four combinations), size (three
combinations), angle from initial starting point (four combinations) and
distance from initial starting point (three combinations). Consequently
there were 144 trials in each block and the entire experiment per
participant comprised a total of 864 trials (six blocks of 144 trials
each). Combinations of target type, distance and angle were presented to
the participant in random sequence with no repetition. Target size was
deliberately set to large for the first forty-eight trials in each block,
followed by medium for the next forty-eight trials, and finally small for
the remaining trials, in order to compensate for learning effects.

The combination of distance and angle from the initial starting point
yielded twelve possible target positions for each trial, as illustrated
in Figure~\ref{fig-target-positions}. Three distances were used that
represented target positions ranging from close to the initial starting
point to very far away from the initial starting point. Four angles were
chosen so that targets could be tested in ninety degree blocks and
giving a good range of screen positions for the target. The first angle
was set to \(45^{\circ}\) with \(90^{\circ}\) increments thereafter, in
order to mimic real life user interface target selection, where targets
are situated in different areas of the screen and therefore selections
are made in multiple directions that are neither simply horizontal nor
vertical.


\begin{figure}
	\centering
	\includegraphics{target-positions}
	\caption{Positions of targets tested. The black box represents the
	initial starting point and the rounded rectangles represent the
	target positions.}
	\label{fig-target-positions}
\end{figure}


The index of difficulty (\(\mathit{ID}_{e}\)) was ascertained for each
possible task using the combination of distance and non-adjusted target
width. This showed that the test had a range of \(\mathit{ID}_{e}\)
values from \unit[0.7]{bits} (\unit[63]{mm} width and \unit[160]{mm}
distance) to \unit[5.4]{bits} (\unit[4]{mm} width and \unit[40]{mm}
distance). It is important to note that the combo box distance values
were adjusted in these calculations to reflect the two-step selection
behaviour of this target type. That is, we need to consider not just the
distance from the initial starting point to the target, but also the
extra distance from the main combo box to the selected list item. In our
experiment, participants were told to always select the third list item
in combo boxes, so the adjusted distance for a combo box was equal to
the normal distance from the initial starting point to the target,
\emph{plus} the additional distance to the third list item.


\subsection{Procedure}
\label{sec-method-procedure}

The participant was initially given an introduction to the test by the
research observer. The introduction included a brief summary of the aims
of the study and what the test involved. The participant was also given
an instruction sheet that they had access to throughout the duration of
the test. After reading the instruction sheet the participant had the
opportunity to ask questions or raise any issues.

Participants were instructed to complete each block of trials as quickly
as possible without losing accuracy. Participants were given the
opportunity to rest for as long as they wished between blocks. It was
made clear to participants that a task was only complete once the target
was successfully selected. Because of the two-step selection behaviour
of the combo box target type, participants were instructed to always
select the third item in the list when selecting a combo box (as
illustrated in Figure~\ref{fig-combo-box}). Additional data about the
selections made on combo boxes were recorded in order to account for the
selection approach of the participant, whether it be a ``one-click'' or
``two-click'' approach.

Before the test began, participants were instructed to complete a
practice session involving fifteen random trials of the same point and
select tasks used in the test. This brought all participants up to a
minimal level of experience with their selection device. This also meant
that each participant knew how to correctly select each target type
including the combo box.

At the conclusion of the test the participant was required to fill out a
questionnaire regarding comfort and user satisfaction with the selection
device used.


\section{Analysis}
\label{sec-analysis}

The data collected from the software included movement time, error rate
and throughput and was used to evaluate selection device performance. A
mixed design repeated measures analysis of variance model (MANOVA) was
used for movement time and throughput to examine within subject
differences in target type and size, as well as between subject
differences in selection device. A Greenhouse and Geisser correction of
the F-ratio was used whenever the Mauchly's test results showed that
assumptions of sphericity were violated.

Post hoc tests, for multiple comparisons, were made using the Bonferroni
method. Due to the skew observed in the error rate data, inter-device
difference in error rates were assessed using the Mann-Whitney U Test.

The comfort questionnaire was based on a five point ordinal scale, where
five generally indicated a bad rating. Because of the small data size, a
Mann-Whitney (non-parametric) test was used. All statistical analyses
were performed using SPSS version 11.0. A \emph{p}-value of \(< 0.05\)
was considered statistically significant.


\section{Results and discussion}
\label{sec-results}


\subsection{Adjusting for learning}
\label{sec-results-learning}

\citet{Doug-SA-1999-CHI} recommend that input device studies should
apply a repeated measures paradigm and test for learning effects. The
effects of learning have been shown to affect movement time and accuracy
\citep{Doug-SA-1999-CHI}.

From analysing the results of movement time and throughput over each
test block, it was clear for the combo box and check box target types
that learning occurred from the first to the second block with the touch
screen, as seen in Figure~\ref{fig-movement-time-learning}. Due to prior
participant experience, no learning was observed with the mouse. No
learning occurred with the text box or button most likely due to their
relatively large size and simple selection behaviour.


\begin{figure}
	\centering
	\includegraphics{combobox-learning}
	\includegraphics{checkbox-learning}
	\caption{Learning is displayed for movement time by selection device
	and test block for the combo box and check box.}
	\label{fig-movement-time-learning}
\end{figure}


Statistical analysis using a simple repeated measure ANOVA was carried
out on movement time for both the check box and combo box. For movement
time of the combo box, the effect of block \(\times\) device was
significant \((F(1.549, 1335.219) = 4.373, p < 0.05)\). Helmhert
contrasts showed that the differences between blocks became
non-significant after block 1 \((p > 0.05)\).

For movement time of the check box, the effect of block \(\times\)
device was significant \((F(1.608, 1385.960) = 4.763, p < 0.05)\). Using
Helmhert contrasts, the differences between blocks became
non-significant after block 1 \((p > 0.05)\), which again shows that
learning occurred in block 1.

To account for learning with the combo and check box, results from block
6 only were used to calculate the movement time and throughput measures,
as the results from block 6 alone gave a good measure of performance.
However, the error rate results were highly skewed for both target
types, with two participants accounting for almost 90\% of the errors.
Error rates for the check box and combo box were therefore calculated
using results from all six test blocks.


\subsection{Movement time}
\label{sec-results-movement}

The results showed that the mouse had an overall movement time of
\unit[1.3]{s} across all target types compared to \unit[1.6]{s} for the
touch screen overlay. Therefore we can conclude that a mouse is on average
15.2\% faster than a touch screen overlay. This is interesting because
\citet{Sear-A-1991-IJMMS} found that the movement times for mouse and
touch screen monitor were similar for rectangular targets larger than
\unit[2]{mm}. Therefore the nature of the two types of touch screen
(overlay versus monitor) may affect the movement time associated with
the type of touch screen. It is also likely that due to the loss of
accuracy found with the overlay during testing, the touch screen monitor
will have faster movement times compared to the touch screen overlay.

The movement times for each target type showed that the text box had the
fastest movement time, followed by the button, the check box and the
combo box. These results are illustrated in
Figure~\ref{fig-movement-time} and are consistent with Fitts' Law
\citep{Fitt-PM-1954-Law}, in that the largest target (the text box) had
the fastest movement time.


\begin{figure}
	\centering
	\includegraphics{movement-time}
	\caption{Movement time for each target type, averaged across both
	devices and all target sizes.}
	\label{fig-movement-time}
\end{figure}


As expected, the combo box had the slowest movement time due to the
two-step behaviour involved in making a selection. The sizes of the
combo box were exactly the same as the text box but movement time was
119\% slower. Thus the extra movement of selecting an item from the
drop-down list increases the movement time involved with the combo box
dramatically. As the distance from the main combo box area to the list
item is relatively short, the significant increase in movement time is
therefore most likely due to users making more errors.

A touch screen overlay has similar movement time to a mouse for the
medium and large targets, but for the small targets, the touch screen
overlay was 67\% slower than the mouse. The only time where the touch
screen overlay was found to be faster than the mouse was with the
largest target type---the large text box.

The movement time for the small check box with the touch screen overlay
was 69\% slower than that of the mouse. The small check box was the
smallest item tested, with dimensions of \unit[4]{mm} \(\times\)
\unit[4]{mm}. We can conclude that the touch screen overlay was not
efficient for selecting targets as small as \unit[4]{mm}. Compare this
with \citet{Sear-A-1991-IJMMS}, who showed that a touch screen has
similar movement time to a mouse for targets as small as \unit[2]{mm}.
While a touch screen monitor can be used with targets as small as
\unit[2]{mm}, a touch screen overlay should only be used for targets
with a size of greater than \unit[4]{mm}. The results from the error
rate analysis also support this conclusion (see
Section~\ref{sec-results-error-rate}).


\subsection{Throughput}
\label{sec-results-throughput}

Throughput for the mouse was \unit[1.238]{bps}, slightly higher than the
\unit[1.215]{bps} throughput for the touch screen overlay. The selection
device by itself was shown not to have a significant effect on
throughput \((F(1, 22) = 0.02, p > 0.05)\). Throughput did not vary for
size but throughput did vary depending on target type \((F(2.07, 45.55)
= 4.77, p < 0.001)\). Check boxes had the highest throughput rate of
\unit[1.967]{bps} (sd = 0.720). This is interesting as the check box was
shown to have the second worst movement time and the worst error rate
(see Figure~\ref{fig-throughput}).


\begin{figure}
	\centering
	\includegraphics{throughput}
	\caption{Throughput for each target type, averaged across both
	devices and all target sizes.}
	\label{fig-throughput}
\end{figure}


Upon further investigation it was seen that the movement time for the
check box was in fact in the middle range of all targets, and due to its
small size it had a high index of difficulty. These two factors are the
most likely reason for the check box having such a high throughput rate.

The combo box had the worst throughput of \unit[0.501]{bps} (sd =
0.213). The index of difficulty was not very high for the combo box, so
its low throughput rate could be attributed to its high movement time.

The overall throughput rate of \unit[1.2]{bps} for the mouse is much
lower than that found by previous research. A study by
\citet{Doug-SA-1994-SIGCHI} showed that a mouse had a throughput rate of
\unit[4.15]{bps}. \citet{Mack-IS-1991} compared three devices (mouse,
tablet and trackball) using four target sizes (8, 16, 32 and 64 pixels)
over two different types of tasks: pointing and dragging. The throughput
for the mouse in this case was \unit[4.5]{bps}. This may indicate that
the level of selection difficulty in our experiment is higher than in
previous research. This could be due to the selection of GUI targets
instead of arbitrary rectangular targets.


\subsection{Error rate}
\label{sec-results-error-rate}

The error rate for the mouse was only 2.7\% which is consistent with
previous studies. The touch screen overlay, on the other hand, had an
error rate of 60.7\%. \citet{Sear-A-1991-IJMMS} found that the touch
screen monitor had an average error rate of 49\% but this was across
much smaller targets. This suggests that there is a loss in accuracy
from using a touch screen overlay compared to a touch screen monitor.

The check box had a significantly high error rate: 78.5\% for all sizes
and both devices and in particular, and 312.5\% for the small check box
with the touch screen overlay. The touch screen overlay incurred the
majority of the errors. With the check box the mouse had an error rate
of 4.4\% and the touch screen overlay had an error rate of 152.5\%. The
distinguishing factor of the check box compared to the other target
types was its small size. We can conclude from this that the touch
screen overlay is more inaccurate for selecting small targets
(\unit[4]{mm} or less).

Buttons and text boxes had much lower error rates compared to that of
the check box and combo box (as seen in Figure~\ref{fig-error-rate}). As
buttons and text boxes also had low movement times, we can conclude that
these two targets have very good overall performance.


\begin{figure}
	\centering
	\includegraphics{error-rate}
	\caption{Error rate for each target type, averaged across both
	devices and all target sizes.}
	\label{fig-error-rate}
\end{figure}


Note that the error rate calculation for combo boxes assumes a
``two-click'' selection approach, as only two of the twenty-four
participants used the ``one-click'' approach. Both of the ``one-click''
participants used the mouse.


\subsection{Comfort}
\label{sec-results-comfort}

In terms of accurate pointing the mouse (2.083) was rated easier than
the touch screen overlay (3.000). These differences were statistically
significant \((p < 0.01)\). The responses regarding the question on
neck, wrist and arm fatigue showed that the touch screen overlay had a
high rating (4.083), whereas the mouse was rated in the midpoint range
(3.167). These differences were statistically significant \((p < 0.5)\).
The final question rated the overall difficulty in using the selection
device. The mouse (4.250) was rated easier to use than the touch screen
overlay (3.333). These differences were statistically significant \((p <
0.05)\).

Participants using the touch screen overlay rated both the text box and
button as easy to accurately select, with the large and medium sizes
being most preferred. This feedback is consistent with the data
collected in that text boxes and buttons have short movement time and
low error rates (i.e., they are easy to accurately select). The combo
box was rated in the middle of the range, and the check box was rated as
very hard to select. Three quarters of the touch screen overlay
participants preferred to select large combo boxes and check boxes,
which is consistent with the poor error rates and movement times
associated with these two target types on the touch screen overlay.

Participants using the mouse rated the text box and button easy to
accurately select, with the large and medium sizes being most preferred.
Both the combo box and check box were rated harder to select than the
button and text box with the check box having the worst rating. As with
the touch screen overlay, participants preferred large combo boxes and
check boxes.

In the general feedback, one participant noted the lack of arm support
for targets at the top of the screen. This is an interesting comment,
because the nature of using a touch screen means the user's arm might be
raised off the desk, and thus be self-supporting when selecting items
towards the top of the screen.

Another suggestion was making the target change colour when the cursor
is located above it. This is a similar concept to that of interactive
rollover items commonly used in web pages. Auditory feedback has been
shown to affect the speed and accuracy when making a selection
\citep{Bend-G-1999-PhD}, and so it likely the visual feedback received
from GUI targets will affect the selection performance. All the targets
that were tested provide some form of immediate visual feedback, from
the button being visually depressed to a tick appearing in the check
box. Further study is needed to assess how visual feedback affects
selection performance of GUI items and what the most effective method of
providing feedback is.


\subsection{Other findings}
\label{sec-results-other}

Calculating the standard deviation of the final selection coordinates on
the screen revealed two interesting patterns. First, when selecting text
boxes with the mouse, there was greater variation in final selection
coordinates on the right side of the screen (\(45^{\circ}\) and
\(315^{\circ}\) target angles) than on the left side of the screen
(\(90^{\circ}\) and \(225^{\circ}\) target angles). This behaviour was
not apparent when selecting text boxes with the touch screen overlay, as
illustrated in Figure~\ref{fig-variation-textbox}. This would mean that
participants using the mouse were much more careful in making selections
with text boxes on the left side of the screen than on the right.


\begin{figure}
	\centering
	\includegraphics{variation-text-mouse}
	\includegraphics{variation-text-touch}
	\caption{Standard deviation of final selection coordinates for the
	text box, averaged across all target sizes.}
	\label{fig-variation-textbox}
\end{figure}


Second, and even more interesting, was the observation that while
selections made on the combo box with the mouse exhibited similar
behaviour to the text box, the behaviour on the touch screen overlay was
more or less the opposite, as shown in
Figure~\ref{fig-variation-combobox}. That is, selections of combo boxes
on the left side of the touch screen overlay had greater variation than
selections made on the right.


\begin{figure}
	\centering
	\includegraphics{variation-combo-mouse}
	\includegraphics{variation-combo-touch}
	\caption{Standard deviation of final selection coordinates for the
	combo box, averaged across all target sizes.}
	\label{fig-variation-combobox}
\end{figure}


These effects were most pronounced for the large size of both the text
box and the combo box. The variation of selection coordinates for the
smaller targets (the button and check box) were consistent across both
the mouse and touch screen overlay and for all target sizes.

We can only speculate as to the causes of this variation. In the case of
the combo box, one possibility is its asymmetrical appearance compared
to the other target types, which may encourage participants to try to
click specifically on the drop-down arrow of the combo box, rather than
clicking on the combo box as a whole. However, this does not explain the
variation between left and right sides of the screen, nor why the same
behaviour was observed with the completely symmetrical text box.

Another possibility is the handedness of the participants, which in the
case of a touch screen might affect how difficult it is to select
targets on different sides of the screen. Unfortunately, we did not ask
participants whether they were right- or left-handed and thus can draw no
conclusions on this point.


\section{Conclusions}
\label{sec-conclusions}

The goal of our study was to assess the efficacy of a touch screen
overlay compared to a mouse, when selecting the typical GUI targets
commonly presented to users in desktop information systems. This was
achieved by an experiment measuring movement time, throughput and error
rate for various combinations of target type, size, angle and distance.
Comfort and user satisfaction were assessed by means of a questionnaire.

The results showed that the touch screen overlay was both slower and
less accurate than the mouse. The touch screen overlay was found to have
reasonable performance with large GUI items but poor performance with
smaller GUI items. The touch screen overlay did have comparable movement
times to the mouse for medium and large sized targets. Throughput did
not vary across device or size but did vary across target type. Both
selection devices had the same user preference except with respect to
the smallest target type---check boxes---in which the mouse had a higher
preference. The mouse was rated easier to make accurate selections with
than the touch screen overlay. The touch screen overlay also had worse
arm, wrist and finger fatigue compared to the mouse. From these results
we can conclude that the mouse had higher user satisfaction than a touch
screen.

An unusual variation in final selection coordinates was noted for both
text boxes and combo boxes. Further studies are required to establish
whether this is a consistent phenomenon and if so, to identify why this
variation occurs.

In general we can conclude that a touch screen overlay with no external
device (e.g., a pen) is not an effective selection device for targets
with dimensions of \unit[4]{mm} or smaller. When designing interfaces
that will be used with a touch screen overlay, selection within the
interface will be more efficient if the GUI items are larger than
\unit[4]{mm}.

Although the results showed that the touch screen overlay was not
efficient and usable for selecting GUI items with a size of \unit[4]{mm}
or less, this may not be the case when a pen or some external device is
used in conjunction with the touch screen overlay. In general there
seems to a lack of research done in device assessment of touch screens
with pens or other external devices. Further testing on touch screens
used with an external device such as a pen may well show that a touch
screen overlay is adequate and efficient for selecting small GUI items.


% The Appendices part is started with the command \appendix;
% appendix sections are then done as normal sections
% \appendix

% \section{}
% \label{}

% Bibliographic references with the natbib package:
% Parenthetical: \citep{Bai92} produces (Bailyn 1992).
% Textual: \citet{Bai95} produces Bailyn et al. (1995).
% An affix and part of a reference:
%   \citep[e.g.][Ch. 2]{Bar76}
%   produces (e.g. Barnes et al. 1976, Ch. 2).

\bibliographystyle{elsart-harv}
\bibliography{IwC_paper}
% \begin{thebibliography}{}

% \bibitem[Names(Year)]{label} or \bibitem[Names(Year)Long names]{label}.
% (\harvarditem{Name}{Year}{label} is also supported.)
% Text of bibliographic item

% \bibitem[]{}
% 
% \end{thebibliography}

\end{document}